Critical period hypothesis: Difference between revisions

From Citizendium
Jump to navigation Jump to search
imported>John Stephenson
(removing all WP material and rewriting)
imported>John Stephenson
(moving to subpages)
Line 16: Line 16:


==Footnotes==
==Footnotes==
{{reflist?2}}
{{reflist|2}}
 
==References==
*Bialystok E & Hakuta K (1994) ''In Other Words: The Science and Psychology of Second Language Acquisition''. New York: HarperCollins.
*Bongaerts T, Planken B & Schils E (1995) Can late learners attain a native accent in a foreign language? A test of the Critical Period Hypothesis. In Singleton D & Lengyel Z (eds) ''The Age Factor in Second Language Acquisition''. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. pp.30-50.
*DeKeyser R (2003) Implicit and explicit learning. In Doughty C & Long MH (eds) ''The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition.'' Malden, MA: Blackwell. pp.313–348.
*Lenneberg EH (1967) ''Biological Foundations of Language''. New York: Wiley.
*Moyer A (1999) 'Ultimate attainment in L2 phonology: the critical factors of age, motivation, and instruction.' ''Studies in Second Language Acquisition'' 21: 81-108.
*Oyama S (1976) 'A sensitive period for the acquisition of a nonnative phonological system.' ''Journal of Psycholinguistic Research'' 5: 261-285.
*Penfield W & Roberts L (1959) ''Speech and Brain Mechanisms.'' Princeton: Princeton University Press.
*Pinker S (1994). ''The Language Instinct.'' New York: Morrow.
*Scovel T (1969) 'Foreign accents, language acquisition, and cerebral dominance.' ''Language Learning'' 19: 245-253.
*[[Martha Young-Scholten|Young-Scholten M]] (2002) Orthographic input in L2 phonological development. In Burmeister P, Piske T & Rohde A (eds) ''An Integrated View of Language Development: Papers in Honor of Henning Wode''. Trier: Wissenschaftlicher Verlag Trier. pp.263-279.
 


==See also==
==See also==
Line 36: Line 23:
*[[First language acquisition]]
*[[First language acquisition]]
*[[Second language acquisition]]
*[[Second language acquisition]]
==Further reading==
*Cook, V. (2001). ''Second Language Learning and Language Teaching''. London: Hodder Arnold.
*Johnson, J.S., and E.L. Newport (1989). 'Critical period effects in second language learning: the influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language.' ''Cognitive Psychology'' 21: 60-99.
* Lamendella, J.T. (1977). 'General principles of Neurofunctional organization and their manifestation in primary and non-primary language acquisition.' ''Language Learning'' 27: 155-159. [''introduces the phrase 'sensitive period'.'']
*Lightbown, P. and Spada, N. (2006). ''How Languages are Learned''. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 2nd edition. [ISBN 0194422240]
*Marshall, Brad. (2000). 'Is there a 'child advantage' in learning foreign languages?' ''Education Week'' 19(22): 39-41.
*Mitchell, R. and Myles, F. (2006). ''Second Language Learning Theories''. London: Hodder Arnold. 2nd edition.
*Newport, E.L. (1990). 'Maturational constraints on language learning.'  ''Cognitive Science'' 14: 11-28.
*Robertson, P. (2002). 'The Critical Age Hypothesis.' ''Asian EFL Journal'': http://www.asian-efl-journal.com/marcharticles_pr.php
*Singleton, David, and Lengyel, Zsolt. (1995). ''The Age Factor in Second Language Acquisition.'' Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. ''See also'' http://www-rcf.usc.edu/~ionin/SLAgroup/Ling527papers/Singleton%20Critical%20Periods%20iral.2005.43.4.269.pdf
*White, L. (2003). ''Second Language Acquisition and Universal Grammar''. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
*Zhao, A.H. and C. Morgan (2005). 'Consideration of Age in L2 Attainment - Children, Adolescents and Adults.' ''Asian EFL Journal'' 6(4): http://www.asian-efl-journal.com/december_04_ahqz_cm.php

Revision as of 06:07, 26 March 2009

This article is developing and not approved.
Main Article
Discussion
Related Articles  [?]
Bibliography  [?]
External Links  [?]
Citable Version  [?]
 
This editable Main Article is under development and subject to a disclaimer.
Language Acquisition
First language acquisition
Second language acquisition
Critical period hypothesis
Contrastive analysis
Fossilization
Applied linguistics
Monitor theory
Language teaching
Communicative approach
Comprehension approach
Multilingualism
Language attrition
Creolistics

The critical period hypothesis (CPH) refers to a long-standing debate in linguistics and language acquisition over the extent to which the ability to acquire language is biologically linked to age. The hypothesis claims that there is an ideal 'window' of time to acquire language in a linguistically rich environment, after which this is no longer possible due to changes in the brain. The hypothesis has been discussed in the context of both first (FLA) and second language acquisition (SLA), and is particularly controversial in the latter. In FLA, it seeks to explain the apparent absence of language in individuals whose childhood exposure was very limited, and in SLA it is often invoked to explain variation in adults' performance in learning a second language, which is very often observed to fall short of nativelike attainment. Various ages have been proposed for the supposed end of the CPH; those that point to pre-adolescent ages such as 12 have been vulnerable to alternative theories which invoke psychological or social factors applying as children move into adolescence.

History

The critical period hypothesis is associated with Wilder Penfield and Lamar Roberts, whose 1959 work Speech and Brain Mechanisms proposed biologically-based limits on the brain that were linked to age. The hypothesis was extended by Eric Lenneberg in his 1967 Biological Foundations of Language, which set the end of the critical period for native language acquisition at 12. The hypothesis has been fiercely debated since then, and has informed popular assumptions about the presumed (in)ability of adults to fluently learn a second language.

In SLA, a weaker version of the CPH emerged in the 1970s. This refers to a sensitive period in which nativelike performance is unlikely but not ruled out.[1] The strongest evidence for the CPH is in the study of accent, where most older learners seem not to reach a native-like level. This leads some researchers to apply the CPH only to second language phonology rather than all aspects of language; indeed, a CPH was not seriously considered for syntax until the 1990s, in research that remains a minority view.[2] However, under certain conditions, native-like accent has been observed, suggesting that accent in SLA is affected by multiple factors, such as identity and motivation, rather than a biological constraint.[3]

First language acquisition

For more information, see: First language acquisition.

Children without language

The CPH as applied to first language acquisition proposes that a child deprived of exposure to natural language would fail to acquire it if exposure commenced only after the end of the critical period. Because testing such a theory would be unethical, in that it would involve isolating a child from the rest of the world for several years, researchers have gathered evidence of the CPH from a few victims of child abuse. The most famous example is the case of Genie (a pseudonym), who was deprived of language until the age of 13. Over the following years of rehabilitation, improvement in her ability to communicate was noted, but during this time she did not develop the language ability common to other children. However, this case has been criticised as a firm example of the critical period in action, and data has not been gathered from Genie since the 1970s.

Although there are several cases on record of deaf children being deprived of sign language, this could also count as abuse. One case in which no abuse took place is that of Chelsea, whose deafness was left undiagnosed until the age of 31. Once hearing aids had apparently restored her hearing to near-normal levels, she seemed to develop a large vocabulary while her phonology and syntax remained at a very low level.[4] The implications of this have been disputed, given the apparently unlikely circumstances of Chelsea's diagnosis.[5]

Footnotes

  1. Oyama (1976).
  2. Scovel (1969) links the CPH mainly to 'foreign accent'; DeKeyser (2003) argues for a critical period in syntax, but only for 'implicit' (subsconscious) learning.
  3. Moyer (1999); Bongaerts et al. (1995); Young-Scholten (2002).
  4. Pinker (1994).
  5. Bialystok & Hakuta (1994).

See also